Teaching and Learning > DISCOURSE
An Electronically Enhanced Philosophical Learning Environment: Who Wants to be Good at Logic?
Author: Susan A.J.Stuart and Margaret Brown
Journal Title: Discourse
ISSN:
ISSN-L: 1741-4164
Volume: 3
Number: 2
Start page: 142
End page: 153
Return to vol. 3 no. 2 index page
Abstract
Over the last academic year we have introduced electronic handsets, like those used on the television show ‘Who wants to be a millionaire?’ (Draper et al. 2002), into the teaching of philosophical logic. Logic lectures can provide quite a formidable challenge for many students, occasionally to the point of making them ill. Our rationale for introducing handsets was threefold: (i) to get the students thinking and talking about the subject in a public environment; (ii) to make them feel secure enough to answer questions in the lectures because the system enabled them to do this anonymously; and (iii) to build their confidence about their learning by their being able to see how they were progressing in relation to the rest of the students in the class. We have achieved all of these and more. Our experience has revealed that the use of handsets encourages a more dynamic form of student interaction in an environment—the lecture—that can, in the wrong hands, be utterly enervating, but they also provide an opportunity for the lecturer to respond to student difficulties at the time when they really matter. In this paper we will discuss our case and why handsets should be introduced, as a model of good pedagogical practice, more widely into the lecturing environment.
Introduction
Of all the topics taught to undergraduate philosophy students philosophical logic can seem to be the most daunting, and not just for the students. Certainly in the Faculty of Arts at the University of Glasgow1 many students give every impression of being maths or symbol phobic and are horrified by the very idea of working with something that can look very much like algebra. They inform you of their dread in the preceding tutorials; they pass on greatly embellished ‘horror’ stories of people who came a cropper in previous years; and then at the end of course in the feedback questionnaires there remains a stalwart few who provide some variation on the theme of “If I had wanted to do Maths, I would have applied for entry to the Science Faculty”. It is true that they face a challenge in the classes that lie before them, and the challenge the lecturer faces is, consequently, no less substantial. Not only does the lecturer have to convince the students that this is a subject they can not only enjoy but succeed in, they have also to overcome the hurdle of the students’ initial resentment to having to do it in the first place. Thus, as you might imagine, being the lecturer to whom the task of teaching logic has fallen is not a much envied position.2
The project that will be described here sprouted from an evaluation study, into the use made by philosophy students of learning resources. In that study we were primarily concerned with three things: (i) finding out which resources offered the best support for good teaching and learning; (ii) carrying out an evaluation of traditional versus non-traditional methods of teaching; and (iii) assessing the relative values of alternative learning resources for teaching formal logic and the acquisition of abstract concepts to non-mathematically oriented students.
Our evaluation of the resources chosen by students followed the model of Integrative Evaluation (Draper et al. 1996), but used as our main instrument ‘Resource Questionnaires’ (Brown et al. 1996). With this tool we were able to measure, using student self-report, which resources students used at all, how much use they made of them, and how much they valued each one. The resources that we considered included lectures, the recommended course text (Tomassi 1999), student-led and non-student-led tutorial discussion, the use of the Personal Response System (PRS) or handsets (Draper et al. 2002) in lectures, electronic texts and electronic course materials, web resources, access to the lecturer, handouts, the Library, and workshops—though there was a category for ‘Other’ for those students who were ingenious enough to discover a resource we had been unable to discover. The evaluator also observed the lectures on occasions when handsets were being used and on the odd occasion when they were not, and we used the handsets to evaluate the advantages and disadvantages of handset use in lectures. Finally, the lecturer provided written and verbal feedback after using the handsets in her lectures.
In this paper we focus only on the use of handsets as a way of creating a more dynamic learning and teaching environment in which students are encouraged to engage with their difficulties and seek to resolve them. Results from the rest of the evaluation are discussed in another paper (Stuart and Brown 2003).
The Scene
There are about 140 students taking the second year philosophy class, and philosophical logic takes up approximately one third of the total lectures for the course. There are four topics in total in the course and students must answer on three of them in the examination, so logic is not compulsory, though taking it is strongly advised for those students who intend to continue into Honours in Philosophy. The class attendance varies in size from about 70 to 100 students. Some of whom will be, very wisely, hedging their bets and coming along to see if they can get the hang of it and, if they can, have another option in the examination.
The handsets were used in nine out of the twelve lectures. They had not been available in previous years, and on this occasion they were only incorporated into the class at the last minute when the lecturer was encouraged to use them by a colleague who was already successfully employing them in a number of other disciplines (Draper & Brown 2003). With a great deal of previous experience of asking questions about how well they were understanding what was being taught and gleaning responses from the usual few conscientious and vocal students who tend to sit towards the front, the lecturer thought that this kind of device would enable her to provoke an entire class into responding to her questions. However, the lecturer did worry that the students might think of them as a novelty and become quickly bored with their intrusive use in the class; as we shall see, her anxiety was unfounded.
The handsets are like television remote control devices and are distributed randomly to every student before the lecture begins. The randomness is important since each handset is numbered and if the same student used the same handset each time, it would be possible to trace the individual responses made by that student and the element of anonymity would be lost. Students are asked multiple choice questions, with up to ten possible answers, that they must think about briefly and respond to fairly quickly, and each student transmits the number corresponding to their chosen answer. The answers are then collected via receivers to a laptop which displays, via the room’s projection system, a bar chart representing the distribution of the responses. In any one lecture it was possible to ask between two and twelve questions, though it should be added that on top of those questions for which the handsets were used the lecturer also asked quick questions that required a ‘hands up’ or verbal response. This was especially important since the class was also being (2-way) video-linked to six students at a remote campus3 and the lecturer did not want those students to feel that this slight difference in provision would make a big difference to their learning. When we used handsets the lecturer asked the students in Crichton to hold up the number of fingers that corresponded to the answer they thought was correct. So their fingers became the equivalent of the numbers 1 to 10 on the handset. When the lecturer asked questions without the use of handsets the lecturer made sure to watch the monitor as well as the people in the lecture room to hear the first correct answer.
The kinds of questions that can be asked vary quite considerably, but typical examples had only two or three possible answers. These were easier to respond to, and more importantly, quicker to ask. Examples included:
- “If the options for the next stage in this proof are MPP or MTT4, which one would you choose? Press 1 for MPP. Press 2 for MTT”.
- “Do you understand this proof?: Press 1 for ‘Yes’. Press 2 for ‘No’.”
- “How would you categorise this statement? Press 1 for ‘Tautology’. Press 2 for ‘Contingency’. Press 3 for ‘Inconsistency’.”
The lecturer did also occasionally ask questions that required a “verbal answer” and then asked the rest of class if they agreed or disagreed, using the handsets to record their response. This had the benefit of being interactive in two ways and worked well once the class had become comfortable with one another and with the handsets. This style of questioning also gave them slightly longer to think about their own answer and the chance to revise their initial response in the light of the answer that had already been given. 5 Once in a while the lecturer asked more complex questions about how well the class felt they were understanding material and, although these sorts of questions were time-consuming and the responses subjective, they were informative enough to provoke her to redirect her teaching. The most interesting of these questions was one in which the lecturer asked them to give her their responses to each one of the ten rules of logic6 they had been taught. They had three possible choices:
- Press 1. for “Dead easy”
- Press 2. for “Difficult but I’m getting there”
- Press 3. for “Dastardly”
The general results were that over a third of the class (39%) found none of them “Dastardly”; that only two students found none of them “Dead Easy”; and only one student found 9 of them “Dastardly”—the exception here was the Double Negation rule which he found “Difficult, but I’m getting there”. However, the most astonishing thing that the lecturer’s question revealed was that many of the students found Disjunctive Syllogism (DS) more difficult to comprehend than V-Elimination (VE). 21.4% of the 56 students who recorded all ten votes found DS more difficult than VE. This was completely unexpected. Disjunctive Syllogism is by far the simpler of the two rules—even, possibly, to the untrained eye, as you might try below. DS states that if you have P or Q and you don’t have P, then you have to have Q, and vice versa.
Disjunctive Syllogism (DS)
The rule of Disjunctive Syllogism states that
given a disjunction: | P v Q |
and the negation of one of the disjuncts: | Q |
we can conclude the other disjunct: | P |
VE states that if Q or P follows from a disjunction of, for example, P or Q, then, because you cannot tell which of P or Q separately or P and Q together it is derived from you must take P by itself and prove Q or P and then take Q by itself and prove Q or P. That way you cannot derive Q or P invalidly.
V-Elimination (VE)
The rule of Disjunction or V-Elimination states that
given a disjunction: | P v Q |
and a conclusion: | Q v P |
we must derive the conclusion from each of the disjuncts separately.
Thus our proof becomes:
1(1) P v Q Assumption
2(2) P Assumption
2(3) Q v P 2 VI (V-introduction)
4(4) Q Assumption
4(5) Q v P 4 VI
1(6) Q v P 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 VE
(The assumptions at lines 2 & 4 are discharged leaving the conclusion
Q v P dependent only on the premise P v Q at line 1.)
The students’ reversal of the difficulty of the two rules was not something the lecturer would have anticipated—perhaps it appeared just deceptively simple and they were reluctant to believe that it could be so easy—but by using the handsets, to enable them to answer questions without the fear of looking ridiculous, the lecturer had the opportunity to go back and adapt her teaching to address a topic that she would not have expected needed to be addressed, at the time that the students needed it. Any other year the lecturer would have had to spend time analysing their examination performance and, on condition that each class of students has exactly the same problems, which in fact we know they do not, act the following year with the benefit of hindsight.
The Benefits of Handsets
One of the essential features of the use of this equipment is that both the students and the lecturer get to know the distribution of responses and, in confidence, how their own response relates to that distribution. The element of anonymity encourages everyone to contribute and, unlike in face to face discussion, each individual can express the choice they incline to rather than the choice they would feel able to explain and justify to others. In other words they provide the student with the ideal circumstances under which they can try out their responses to questions without any fear of embarrassment if their answer turns out not to be right. This selection of student comments confirms this:
- The anonymity allows the student to show they’re unsure of the subject without embarrassing themselves.
- It’s anonymous—tend to be more honest! Also can compare answer to other people’s which can be very reassuring!
- Compensates for lack of confidence, provides anonymity.
- Encourages us to participate; more likely we will listen this way.
- People answer more willingly.
- Students see how well they understand the material and compare their progress with others.
- Easier to hold attention. Fun!
When students were asked their opinion of the usefulness of handsets in their lectures, 77% rated them useful / very useful / extremely useful.
What we have, then, is a new resource that changes the form of a very old resource, the lecture. Traditionally lectures have been a period of time in which the student is a passive auditor and is being provided with knowledge from the person addressing them at the front. During the lecture students can drift in and out of awareness of what is being said, and a reflection of this drift can be seen in the recurrent patchiness of most student lecture notes. However, even in an apparently excellent interactive lecture, where the lecturer is attempting to engage the attention of the students by asking questions, there will be just a few students who actually speak with the rest keeping quiet, and consequently, the state of understanding of most of the class remains, at least, reasonably opaque until the examination has come and gone. But, where handsets are being used effectively—engaging the best students along with those who might be struggling—they facilitate an interaction between students and lecturer that keeps the students thinking and concentrating on the material throughout the lecture7.
In informal interviews several students had said that they were more likely to try and work out the answer to a question if handsets were being used. So, towards the end of the course we asked all students in the class if looking back over the course when they were given a problem to work out in a lecture, were they more likely to work out the answer if they were asked to answer verbally, “hands up” or using handsets. The result of this question which was asked using handsets is shown in Fig. 1.
Question number | Option Given a problem to work out in a lecture, were you more likely to work out the answer if: |
% of students who voted for each option |
1 | the class was asked for a verbal response | 0% |
2 | the class was asked to vote on one or more answers by putting their hand up | 2% |
3 | the class was asked to vote on one or more answers using the handsets | 32% |
4 | none of the above (i.e. I never try to work out an answer) | 6% |
5 | all of the above (i .e. I always try to work an answer out) | 28% |
6 | 1 and 2 (i.e. verbal and hands up but not handsets) | 2% |
7 | 1 and 3 (i.e. verbal and handsets, but not hands up) | 4% |
8 | 2 and 3 (i.e. hands up and handsets, but not verbal) | 26% |
Fig. 1 Preferred method of answering questions in a lecture
Such results can only be interpreted as the students enjoying the interactive nature of the lectures, with their responses providing us with an overwhelming endorsement of the use of handsets to engage students and keep their attention. And, if they are more likely to work out the answer when they are using handsets, then this can only benefit their learning.
One of the most interesting things to come out of this study was that if the students were to answer the questions in ways that would be beneficial to them, that is, if they are not just to guess, then they have to reflect more on what they have learnt and how they are learning. A number of students reported that this was indeed the case when handsets were used, but also that when they could see how well they were doing they felt much more confident about what, up to then, they had only thought they knew. It was also clear that the opportunity for students to think about their answers and discuss them with their neighbour also made a difference to their understanding. It seems likely that this was the result of having to generate arguments for and against alternative answers. There is no doubt that having to do this is a powerful promoter of learning, and unquestionably beneficial to the development of critical thinking skills. The conclusion here can only be that this is an excellent way in which to carry out formative assessment. And, of course, if this formative assessment is working well, the students will be in a strong position to let the lecturer know about the gaps in their knowledge, enabling the lecturer to turn their attention to what the students say they need, rather than what they think they need. Which, in turn, means that the lecturer is much less likely to try to second-guess or make unwarranted assumptions about the students’ progress. However, none of this is valuable unless the lecturer is flexible enough to respond to the changing requirements of the class; an adaptability described rather aptly, since it is so far from the traditional idea of delivering a lecture and leaving the room, by Draper & Brown 2003 as ‘contingent teaching’.
A Contrasting Case
But, before we finish with only positive conclusions, we should mention the one or two problems that we encountered. The handsets, along with their infra-red receivers (and we needed three for the size of the lecture room), the visualiser or OHP, two screens (one to show the numbers of the handsets of those students who had successfully transmitted and recorded their vote8 and one to show the question), and the PRS software on the lecture theatre computer, were all necessary and, unless you have a lecture theatre that is already fully equipped for PRS use, setting up can be a time-consuming job at the beginning of a lecture, or even in the ten minute break between lectures. The only complaint we had from students in the Logic lectures was that the time to set the system up and running did occasionally eat into the lecture time and, if the lecturer started even though things were not quite ready, they found the setting-up that was going on around her disruptive. However, on the one occasion that there were significant difficulties setting up the equipment they were local to the design of the particular lecture theatre and not PRS. The ideal situation would be one in which lecture theatres had the system built in to its functioning, so they could be used at very short notice by anyone taking a class in the room.
The only other problem that the lecturer experienced in the use of handsets was when she decided to try them out in a first year Philosophy of Mind lecture. The experience was not positive. The lecturer had too much material to get through with the class on that day and reported having felt herself becoming tense when there was a hold up getting the PRS system to work. Even more unfortunately the lecturer had not taken the handsets into account properly and had not realised that the sorts of questions she would be able to ask would be very different from the rather clear-cut questions that can be asked in a logic class. As a result she asked enormously subjective questions like "Do you feel that you have understood Behaviourism?", and found herself in the position of not being able to offer more clarity because of the limited amount of time available. This time limit also meant that she failed to discuss the voting with the students or even leave the charts up for long enough for them to look at. But when the students were asked if they had enjoyed using the handsets they looked rather non-plussed but gallantly replied ‘Yes’.
Conclusions
Students report that lectures are an important resource in logic and this is borne out by the examination results where we compared attenders with non-attenders. The lowest score for attenders was 30% and the highest score was 100%; the lowest score for non-attenders was 5% and the highest score was 95%, with a cluster of lower scores also being from the non-attenders. From the lecturer’s perspective handsets are an important new tool, especially in larger classes where it can be impossible to get everyone to respond. The lecturer can obtain immediate feedback about what the students think they know and understand and, subsequently, they can redirect their teaching based on what the students feel weak on rather than on what they predict the students will find difficult. The students in the logic class did not regard the handsets as a novelty, become bored with them and find them intrusive. The students in the Philosophy of Mind class did not have the opportunity to become bored with them and, if the lecturer had thought more carefully about the sorts of information she had wanted from them, they would not have had the opportunity to think of them as intrusive. The lecturer has no doubt that if she had taken time and planned ahead on that occasion, she would have learnt a great deal which would have helped her pitch her lectures and address or re-address aspects of the course that, by now, she had begun to take for granted.
Even with the problems we have mentioned it is possible to conclude that using handsets competently in lectures does engage students and encourages a much more dynamic form of student-lecturer and student-student interaction. Handsets enable all students, weak and strong, to think, to answer (anonymously) and get immediate feedback on their knowledge and understanding. They can see how well they are doing in relation to others and they report feeling more confident about what, up to then, they only thought they knew. If this tool is to be really helpful to the students they must reflect more on what they have learnt and how they are learning. We have found that they do. Using handsets has made it possible to provide an interesting and exciting way for students to gain some insight into their progress and, in this particular case, to develop the critical thinking skills that are fundamental to thinking philosophically.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the LTSN Philosophy and Religious Studies Subject Centre, University of Leeds, for the mini-project funding with which we have carried out this work. We would also like to thank Dr Steve Draper9 from the Department of Psychology, University of Glasgow, for his ideas, assistance and encouragement. In addition we would also like to acknowledge the GRUMPS10 project for giving us permission to use their handsets.
Endnotes
- Students at the University of Glasgow enter a Faculty rather than a specific Department; they have to do three subjects in their first and second (pre-Honours) years, and thus may be doing Philosophy as a minor class before they go on to Honours in a different subject or taking Philosophy as a necessary component of a three year non-Honours degree. Only 50% of the second year class will intend taking Philosophy to Honours level.
- I volunteered; too many students were disillusioned by their experience and I was sure that I could make the comprehension of logic less of an ordeal for them.
- Crichton Campus, a remote campus of the University of Glasgow at Dumfries.
- MPP is an abbreviation for Modus Ponendo Ponens and means to affirm the antecedent; thus if you have two propositions: ‘If A then B’, and A, then you can conclude B. MTT is an abbreviation for Modus Tollendo Tollens and means to deny the consequent; thus if you have two propositions: ‘If A then B’, and you have the denial of B, you can then conclude the denial of A.
- We discovered that quite quickly the students were in a position to identify the usual people giving the verbal responses and gauge their own follow-up responses on the consistency of their responses. So this method became one that could be used only when the conscientious responders were not present, which was very rare, or when they had been asked not to be the first to answer, something I did not like to do because it might stifle their enthusiasm.
- We had by this stage covered the rules of Assumption, Double Negation, MPP, MTT, &-introduction, &-elimination, v-introduction, Disjunctive Syllogism, v-elimination, and Conditional Proof.
- To encourage student-student interaction students were occasionally asked to answer a question without thinking about it for too long; they were then were asked to discuss it with their neighbour and answer it again. The shift towards a greater number giving the right answer on the second attempt was sometimes quite extraordinary, and it seems that the students who had a better grasp of the subject were able to convince their neighbour to change their mind and vote differently the second time. This is certainly one way to promote discussion of a subject like logic that most students would prefer to avoid.
- Students had only to check the number on the back of the particular handset they had picked from the box that day and look at the screen to see if their vote had been transmitted and recorded.
- For a fine array of other handset use see: http://www.psy.gla.ac.uk/~steve/ilig/
- GRUMPS is an acronym for Generic Remote User Measurement Production System. GRUMPS is a EPSRC-DIM funded project in GU developing techniques and software to collect, manage and analyse large collections of user action data. Documents: http://grumps.dcs.gla.ac.uk/
Bibliography
- Brown, M.I., Doughty, G.F., Draper, S.W., Henderson, F.P., & McAteer, E., ‘Measuring Learning Resource Use’, Computers and Education 27, pp103–113, 1996
- Draper, S.W., Brown, M.I., Henderson, F.P., McAteer E., ‘Integrative evaluation: An emerging role for classroom studies of CAL’, Computers and Education 26, no1-3, pp17-32, 1996
- Draper, S.W., Cargill, J., & Cutts, Q., ‘Electronically enhanced classroom interaction’ Australian Journal of Educational Technology, 18, no. 1, pp13-23, 2002. Also available at http://staff.psy.gla.ac.uk/~steve/ilig/handsets.html
- Draper, S.W. & Brown, M.I., ‘Increasing Interactivity in Lectures Using an Electronic Voting System’, 2003, (Under Review)
- Stuart, S.A.J. & Brown, M.I., ‘An Evaluation of Learning Resources in the Teaching of Formal Philosophical Methods’, 2003 (Under Review)
- Tomassi, P., Logic, (Routledge Kegan Paul, 1999)
Return to vol. 3 no. 2 index page
This page was originally on the website of The Subject Centre for Philosophical and Religious Studies. It was transfered here following the closure of the Subject Centre at the end of 2011.